"

Chapter 3: Visual Pathways

3.8. Mirror Neurons

We have seen that perceiving motion is an important part of the visual system. We also have a specialized set of mirror neurons that fire both when a person observes another individual performing a movement (e.g., smiling) and when they perform the same movement themselves. These neurons were first discovered in monkeys’ brains. In humans, mirror neurons are thought to exist in the premotor cortex, the supplementary motor area, the primary somatosensory cortex, and the inferior parietal cortex (Figure 3.13).

An outline of the brain showing SMA near the front and PSSC near the middle.
Figure 3.13 The Mirror Neuron System in the Human Brain. (1) SMA: supplementary motor area, (2) PSSC: primary somato sensory cortex, (3) IPC: Inferior parietal cortex, (4) VPMA: ventral premortal area, neurons having mirror properties, BA: Broca’s area, WA: Wernicke’s area, FG: fusiform gyrus, AG: angular gyrus, PMC: primary motor cortex. (Provided by Acharya and Shukla, JNSBM. License: CC BY 3.0).

It has been suggested that mirror neurons help us to understand actions and intentions of other people, and facilitate imitation, language, and empathy.

One of the fundamental arguments surrounding the discovery of mirror neurons lies in the fact that these neurons help understand the motor acts of others in an immediate way, without being mediated by a reflexive-conceptual process. This type of understanding of intentionality is obtained based on the acts perceived in others through motor resonance in the perceiver, which in turn enables the understanding of motor intentions as emotional states. Thus, the understanding of perceived motor acts arises first at the motor and emotional level rather than at the reflexive and conceptual level. More details about the connection between mirror neurons and emotions/empathy can be found in the RACC article (Brunsteins, 2011).

However, the question of what the function is of the mirror neuron system is probably an ill-posed question. Mirror neurons do not have a unique functional role. Their properties indicate, rather, that they represent a mechanism that maps the pictorial description of actions carried out in the higher order visual areas onto their motor counterpart. This matching mechanism may underlie a variety of functions. More details about specific functions served by mirror neurons are in this Scholarpedia article.

Learn more about mirror neurons in the video linked here and included below.

 

 

Much of the material from Chapter 3 comes from:

Dommett, E. (2023). Lighting the world: our sense of vision. In C. Hall (Ed). Introduction to biological psychology. University of Sussex. https://doi.org/10.20919/ZDGF98294

Some of the materials come from Olman, C. & the Class of 3031 at the University of Minnesota. https://pressbooks.umn.edu/sensationandperception/
Some comes from: Grose-Fifer, J., Spielman, R. M., Dumper, K., Jenkins, W., Lacombe, A., Lovett, M., & Perlmutter, M. (2021). Introduction to Psychology (A critical approach). CUNY Pressbooks. https://pressbooks.cuny.edu/jjcpsy101/

 

References

Acharya, S., & Shukla, S. (2012). Mirror neurons: Enigma of the metaphysical modular brain. Journal of Natural Science, Biology, and Medicine, 3(2), 118–124. https://doi.org/10.4103/0976-9668.101878

Allison, T., Puce, A., & McCarthy, G. (2000). Social perception from visual cues: Role of the STS region. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 4(7), 267-278. https://doi.org/10.1016/s1364-6613(00)01501-1

Amoruso, L., Couto, B., & Ibáñez, A. (2011). Beyond Extrastriate Body Area (EBA) and Fusiform Body Area (FBA): Context Integration in the Meaning of Actions. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 5, 124. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2011.00124

Aronoff, M. (2007). Language. Scholarpedia, 2(5), 3175.

Balas, B., & Saville, A. (2017). Hometown size affects the processing of naturalistic face variability. Vision Research, 141, 228-236.

Bartfeld, E., & Grinvald, A. (1992). Relationships between orientation-preference pinwheels, cytochrome oxidase blobs, and ocular-dominance columns in primate striate cortex. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA, 89, 11905-11909.

Benson, N. C., Yoon, J. M., Forenzo, D., Engel, S. A., Kay, K. N., & Winawer, J. (2022). Variability of the surface area of the V1, V2, and V3 maps in a large sample of human observers. Journal of Neuroscience, 42(46), 8629-8646.

Brunsteins, P. (2011). El Rol de la Empatía en la Atribución Mental [The Role of Empathy in Mental Attribution]. Revista Argentina de Ciencias Del Comportamiento, 3(1), 75–84.

Chariker, L., Shapley, R., Hawken, M., & Young, L. S. (2022). A computational model of direction selectivity in Macaque V1 cortex based on dynamic differences between ON and OFF pathways. Journal of Neuroscience, 42(16), 3365-3380.

Conway, B. R., Kitaoka, A., Yazdanbakhsh, A., Pack, C. C., & Livingstone, M. S. (2005). Neural basis for a powerful static motion illusion. Journal of Neuroscience, 25(23), 5651-5656.

Fernández-Vigo, J. I., Burgos-Blasco, B., Calvo-González, C., Escobar-Moreno, M. J., Shi, H., Jiménez-Santos, M., Valverde-Megías, A., Reche-Frutos, J., López-Guajardo, L., & Donate-López, J. (2021). Assessment of vision-related quality of life and depression and anxiety rates in patients with neovascular age-related macular degeneration. Archivos de la Sociedad Española de Oftalmología (English Edition), 96(9), 470-475. https://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.oftale.2020.11.008

Filimon, F., Nelson, J. D., Huang, R. S., & Sereno, M. I. (2009). Multiple parietal reach regions in humans: cortical representations for visual and proprioceptive feedback during on-line reaching. Journal of Neuroscience, 29(9), 2961-2971.

Foroni, F., Pergola, G., & Rumiati, R. I. (2016). Food color is in the eye of the beholder: the role of human trichromatic vision in food evaluation. Scientific Reports, 6(1), 37034. https://doi.org/10.1038/srep37034

Freeman, A. W. (2021). A model for the origin of motion direction selectivity in visual cortex. Journal of Neuroscience, 41(1), 89-102.

Griffith, A. N., Hurd, N. M., & Hussain, S. B. (2019). “I didn’t come to school for this”: A qualitative examination of experiences with race-related stressors and coping responses among Black students attending a predominantly White institution. Journal of Adolescent Research34(2), 115-139.

Jenkins, R., White, D., Van Montfort, X., & Burton, A. M. (2011). Variability in photos of the same face. Cognition, 121(3), 313-323.

Kelly, D. J., Quinn, P. C., Slater, A. M., Lee, K., Ge, L., & Pascalis, O. (2007). The other-race effect develops during infancy: Evidence of perceptual narrowing. Psychological Science, 18(12), 1084-1089.

Lee, J., & Penrod, S. D. (2022). Three-level meta-analysis of the other-race bias in facial identification. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 36( 5), 1106– 1130. https://doi.org/10.1002/acp.3997

McKone, E., Wan, L., Pidcock, M., Crookes, K., Reynolds, K., Dawel, A., Kidd, E., & Fiorentini, C. (2019). A critical period for faces: Other-race face recognition is improved by childhood but not adult social contact. Scientific Reports, 9(1), 1–13. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-019-49202-0

NICE. (2022). Cataracts. Retrieved from https://cks.nice.org.uk/topics/cataracts/background-information/causes-risk-factors/

Pascolini, D., & Mariotti, S. P. (2012). Global estimates of visual impairment: 2010. British Journal of Ophthalmology, 96(5), 614-618. https://doi.org/10.1136/bjophthalmol-2011-300539

Pellegrini, M., Bernabei, F., Schiavi, C., & Giannaccare, G. (2020). Impact of cataract surgery on depression and cognitive function: Systematic review and meta-analysis. Clinical & Experimental Ophthalmology, 48(5), 593-601. https://doi.org/10.1111/ceo.13754

Pöppel, E., Held, R., & Frost, D. (1973). Residual visual function after brain wounds involving the central visual pathways in man. Nature, 243(5405), 295-296.

Purves, D., Augustine, G. J., Fitzpatrick, D., Katz, L. C., LaMantia, A.-S., McNamara, J. O., & Williams, S. M. (Eds.). (2001). The functional organization of extrastriate visual areas. In Neuroscience (2nd ed.). Sinauer Associates. https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/books/NBK10884/

Quaranta, L., Riva, I., Gerardi, C., Oddone, F., Floriani, I., & Konstas, A. G. (2016). Quality of life in glaucoma: A review of the literature. Advances in Therapy, 33(6), 959-981. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12325-016-0333-6

Rizzolatti, G., & Destro, M. F. (2008). Mirror neurons. Scholarpedia, 3(1), 2055.

Vaina, L. M. (1998). Complex motion perception and its deficits. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 8(4), 494-502. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0959-4388(98)80037-8

Weiskrantz, L. (1999). Consciousness lost and found: A neuropsychological exploration. OUP. https://doi.org/10.1093/acprof:oso/9780198524588.001.0001

Weiskrantz, L., & Warrington, E. K., Sanders, M. D., & Marshall, J. (1974). Visual capacity in the hemianopic field following a restricted occipital ablation. Brain, 97(1), 709-728. https://doi.org/10.1093/brain/97.1.709

World Health Organisation. (2018). Blindness and visual impairment. Retrieved from https://www.who.int/news-room/fact-sheets/detail/blindness-and-visual-impairment

World Health Organisation. (2021). Global eye care targets endorsed by member states at the 74th World Health Assembly. Retrieved from https://www.who.int/news/item/27-05-2021-global-eye-care-targets-endorsed-by-member-states-at-the-74th-world-health-assembly

Yin, R. K. (1969). Looking at upside-down faces. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 81, 141-145.

Zeki, S. (1980). The representation of colours in the cerebral cortex. Nature, 284, 412-418. https://doi.org/10.1038/284412a0

Zeki, S., & Marini, L. (1998). Three cortical stages of colour processing in the human brain. Brain, 121(9), 1669-1685. https://doi.org/10.1093/brain/121.9.1669

License

Icon for the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License

Sensation and Perception Copyright © 2025 by Dr. Jill Grose-Fifer; Students of PSY 3031; and Edited by Dr. Cheryl Olman is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, except where otherwise noted.